On the other hand, a shorter instrument will have a higher drone which requires your lips to vibrate at a higher frequency. Apart from the basic drone, a good didgeridoo player is also able to produce a variety of other sounds in order to create rhythms and music, instead of a plain continuous drone.
This is done using various techniques. Animal sounds or calls are done using the vocal cords whereby the didgeridoo player creates the drone with vibrating lips whilst also talking, yelling or screaming into the instrument. Popular animal sounds include bird calls and dingo yelps, in fact, you can imitate any animal you like by playing the drone and using your voice to make those animal sounds at the same time.
The vocal cords can also be used in another way, which is to passively allow them to vibrate at a low frequency. Instead of the high-pitch animal calls, you get a low hum from passively using your voice. This hum is lower than the pitch of the basic drone and adds another dimension to the overall sound of the didgeridoo. Other sounds are produced with the tongue or by varying the degree of "puffiness" of your cheeks. Move your tongue around whilst you are playing the basic drone and you'll notice a slight change in sound.
A good player is able to move the tongue deliberately to create certain changes in harmonics or to produce rhythmic patterns. Try puffing your cheeks in and out and you will also notice subtle changes in harmonics and timbre. In addition to using the vocal cords, tongue and cheeks to vary the sound of the didgeridoo and to make it more interesting, the lips can also be used to create variety by either tensing or loosening them. A trumpet note is produced when you purse your lips tightly, and an accomplished didgeridoo is able to "hit" two or more of these trumpet notes, each with its own distinctive pitch.
The opposite effect is created by loosening the lips slightly so that the basic drone note drops a little. In this way, an astonishing variety of sounds can be produced on a didgeridoo, and when aided by circular breathing, allows you to play continuously without a break for as long as you like.
Circular breathing is a technique used by didgeridoo players to produce an continuous sound. This is done by playing an interrupted sound on the didgeridoo whilst occasionally breathing in through the nose.
The pitch or note of a didgeridoo is simply what key the drone plays in. This can be measured easily and quickly with a guitar or chromatic tuner. Almost all didgeridoos are found in the following key range. These instruments however require more air and effort to play them well. The lower keys are often more sensitive on the mouthpiece requiring greater lip control. They often have less sensitivity on the mouthpiece which allows them to be played with greater ease.
These instruments however also require greater lip control often in the form of tighter lips and a player needs greater control in technique and timing. The reduced lip control required to play them and the versatility of being in the mid range allows them to be usually played fast or slow As each didgeridoo is unique with its own set of characteristics and features it is still quite feasible to find instruments leaning towards the lower or higher keys that are still good beginners instruments.
The above key guide is a sensible approach to choosing a first didgeridoo but by no means a hard and fast rule. A traditional didgeridoo, typically called yidaki , mago or more recently mandapul, is one which originates from one of the several distinct regions in Arnhem Land in the Northern Territory of Australia. The best known of these would be the Yidaki. The word yidaki is applied to traditional didgeridoos from North East Arnhem Land that are made by Yolngu people that often have distinct differences, acoustically and structurally, that set them apart from standard didgeridoos.
They are decorated with natural earth pigments ochres that represent special clan designs and motifs. The vocal tract is a resonator that, in normal speech, can assist the radiation of some frequency bands, but not others. In fact its resonances are what allow us to produce different speech sounds: see voice acoustics for an introduction. For a didjeridu player, the vocal tract is working backwards: it still has resonances, but the vibration is usually coming from the lips, rather than from the vocal folds.
In either speech or in didjeridu playing, the frequencies at which the vocal tract resonates are determined by the shape of the tract, especially by the position and shape of the tongue, and the state of closure of the vocal folds.
In the spectrogram at right, Lloyd Hollenberg illustrates several of the effects discussed above. The horizontal yellow line at the bottom of the spectrogram is the drone at constant frequency. The other horizontal lines are harmonics.
The larger patches of light colour — the ones that vary — are formants. These are changing with time as Lloyd changes the shape of his tract. Download didj sound file in. In a few places, he vocalises: that is, his vocal folds vibrate while he is playing. In this case, the vocal tract is being driven by vibrations at both ends, by the lip vibration and the vocal fold vibration.
The frequencies are different, and the result is not just two pitches, but also notes with pitches corresponding to the sum of the two frequencies, and others as well.
Details in the experimental paper. This process is difficult because it is so noisy inside a wind player's mouth — over dB. That's because the vibrating lips transmit a sound in both directions: into the instrument, and also into the vocal tract. The waves that go into the vocal tract interact with its resonances, and then some frequencies pass into the instrument to emerge in the output sound. To produce the strong formants characteristic of didjeridu performance, we found that the player produces two or more strong peaks in the impedance spectrum.
These inhibit sound at their frequencies, and the uninhibited frequencies in between are heard as a formant in the sound. For instance, with the tongue very near the roof of the mouth, one can strongly inhibit the frequencies around 1.
This results in a strong isolated band around 1. See the technical page for spectra, sound files and film clips. Our hearing is very good at identifying formants in the speech range, so we are very conscious of their presence, especially when they change over time as the player moves his tongue. A virtuoso like William Barton recent soloist with the London Philharmonic and Sydney Symphony can produce an amazing range of sounds.
In one of our previous papers, we measured the acoustic response of players' vocal tracts while they mimed playing, and that certainly gave clues about the mechanism. But miming does not come naturally to players of this instrument, and we were especially uncertain about the position of the players' vocal folds when miming.
Hence the importance of the measurements during performance. The yidaki or didjeridu is an iconic Australian instrument and, as an Australian research lab specialising in music acoustics, we thought that we should.
Further, this instrument demonstrates the most spectacular case of the vocal tract influencing the sound. By understanding it, we hope to understand vocal tract effects in other instruments , where the effects are much smaller, but still musically important. Then there is curiosity. It is an unusual system with some subtle features and some interesting physics. Science and technology often benefit in unexpected ways from researchers looking into questions just from curiosity.
Vocalisations are much more common in modern styles than in traditional performance. When the vocal folds vibrate during play, the vocal tract is driven by two vibrations, at different frequencies, from the lip vibration and the vocal fold vibration. The result is not just two pitches, but a complicated set of frequencies corresponding to the sum and difference of the two frequencies, and others as well.
We discuss the physics of circular breathing and vocalisation in this paper and the effects of vocalisation in this paper. None of the players to whom we have spoken are aware of it, except during vocalisations.
Did your method of sound measurement impair the players' ability to play? Many players play with the instrument a bit to one side of the mouth. It was easier for such players to deal with our system. This is a subtle question and there are several subtle effects.
However, there is one over-riding effect: players prefer instruments that do not have very strong resonances in the range kHz. This is easy to understand in terms of the story that we tell above: this frequency range is the one in which players can manipulate the formants using the resonances of their own vocal tracts. They prefer instruments whose resonances that fall in this range are weak compared to the resonances of the vocal tract. This is explained in more detail in this paper. Such studies are difficult because they involve detecting the probe sound amid the noise of the didgeridoo's drone.
Sound levels inside the player's mouth can reach decibels, which is as loud as a chainsaw. Wolfe and his team discovered that formants are produced when the player closes the glottis, which narrows the windpipe.
If the vocal tract remains fully open, as in normal breathing, the lungs absorb much of the sound, the researchers report in Nature1. So have they discovered a fast track to expert didgeridoo playing? It will still take practice to emulate the most sophisticated players, Wolfe answers, not least because skilled playing requires a mastery of circular breathing.
This involves maintaining the outward airflow through the mouth by contracting the cheeks while breathing in through the nose. Try it and you'll see how tricky it is Learning to make strong formants takes a while. Other techniques involve vocalizing and playing at the same time: one gets interactions between the vibrations from the lips and from the vocal cords.
Nature , Download references. You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar. Physics news. Physics of the didgeridoo. University of New South Wales music acoustics. Reprints and Permissions. Hopkin, M.
0コメント